SitNews - Stories in the News - Ketchikan, Alaska

ALASKA SCIENCE

Renowned raven researcher visits Alaska
By NED ROZELL

 

May 06, 2009
Wednesday


Some people are great runners. Some people write good books. Some follow their curiosity to figure out something no one else has.

Bernd Heinrich is all of the above. The University of Vermont professor, bestselling author, and record-setting ultra marathoner traveled to Fairbanks recently at the invitation of an old friend, George Happ of the Institute of Arctic Biology.

jpg Bernd Heinrich

Bernd Heinrich is a bestselling author and raven expert. He traveled to Fairbanks recently and provided a lecture at the University of Alaska Fairbanks campus.
Photo by Ned Rozell.

jpg raven

Bernd Heinrich has written several books on ravens and has studied the birds for decades in the Maine woods.
Photo courtesy the U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service.

While here, Heinrich set up a tray of slides and spoke about one of his favorite subjects, ravens, to an audience that had just endured a long winter elbow to wingtip with the black birds.

"Coming to Alaska and talking about ravens is like hauling coal to Newcastle," he said.

Heinrich, 67, wore a flannel shirt and running shoes, and had the light-footed-yet-rugged appearance of someone who could run loops on a track until he had completed 100 miles (which took him 12 hours and 27 minutes in 1984). He also once won the Golden Gate Marathon in San Francisco, and was that same year the top Men's Masters (40 and older) finisher out of thousands at the Boston Marathon.

His editors at large publishing houses might not be aware of his running prowess, but they know of his books: "Bumblebee Economics," "Insect Thermoregulation," "In a Patch of Fireweed," "One Man's Owl," "Ravens in Winter," "An Owl in the House," "The Hot-Blooded Insects," "A Year in the Maine Woods," "The Thermal Warriors," "The Trees in My Forest," "Mind of the Raven," "Racing the Antelope (Why We Run)," "The Winter World," "The Geese of Beaver Bog," "The Snoring Bird," "The Summer World."

For a hint of why several of Heinrich's books have landed on The New York Times Best Seller list, see the non-technical titles of his scientific journal articles, such as "How do bees shiver?" and "Do common ravens yell because they want to attract others?"

In Fairbanks, Heinrich spoke of how yelling ravens at a moose carcass in Maine sparked his interest in the birds. Why would any creature shout out the location of its food supply?

He went about finding the answer to that and other raven questions with uncommon effort. Dairy farmers donated dead cows and calves to Heinrich, which he would use to attract ravens during his research in the Maine woods. He then recorded calls of ravens and played them back to them. He climbed to raven nests and inserted pipe cleaners down the throats of new ravens to make them vomit venison and reveal what their parents fed them. Heinrich and his students once caught 43 ravens in one shot with a large trap, and he once dressed in a homemade wolf costume to see how ravens would react.

"They were even more afraid of me," Heinrich said.

He also built an "aviary" in the Maine woods in which he held many wild ravens and learned volumes about their behavior. From dozens of observations and experiments in the aviary, he determined that the yell ravens emit around a food source is "probably derived from the begging of juveniles in the nest." It is the call of younger birds that desire food, but are still submissive to adult birds.

Many Alaskans have noticed ravens commuting in and out of cities and towns in the winter. They are heading to and from communal roosts, often a remote copse of spruce trees in which the ravens perch during the night like black Christmas ornaments. Heinrich once performed an experiment to determine if these roosts were information centers, where ravens would somehow relate the location of a cow carcass, or a Dunkin Donuts' trash bin.

From his aviary, Heinrich would coax a raven into a pet carrier; carry it into the woods to a roost he had located, and release the bird, which would then join with the roosting birds. In 10 out of 10 cases, his raven showed up where the roosting ravens were feeding. If he released a raven away from the roosting site, just one out of 10 ravens would find the bonanza upon which the wild roosting birds were feeding. Somehow the ravens were communicating, but maybe not by talking about food. He could imagine birds following the early risers that took off in a rush to the feeding site, and there was the transfer of information.

"If somebody takes off eagerly in a certain direction, that's the place to go," he said.

 

This column is provided as a public service by the Geophysical Institute,
University of Alaska Fairbanks, in cooperation with the UAF research community.
Ned Rozell [
nrozell@gi.alaska.edu] is a science writer at the institute.

 

E-mail your news & photos to editor@sitnews.us


Publish A Letter in SitNews         Read Letters/Opinions

Contact the Editor

SitNews ©2009
Stories In The News
Ketchikan, Alaska